Alcohol junk science promoters and believers try to influence laws and public policy. For example, the New York State Assembly‘s Committee on Alcoholism and Drug Abuse held hearings. It wanted to know whether or not alcohol ads has an effect on youthful drinking. And, if so, what action the Assembly should take.
The Faithful

For most who testified, it was an article of faith. Alcohol ads cause young people to drink. And calls for strong action. They converged on the hearing like the faithful assembling for a tent revival meeting. And their testimony was about as based on science as the rhetoric at a religious revival.
Research from around the world has repeatedly shown for decades that alcohol ads don’t increase overall drinking. They don’t contribute to alcohol abuse. And they don’t cause non-drinkers to become drinkers. But successful advertisers increase their market share. But it’s at the expense of their competitors, who lose market share.
But scientific evidence was irrelevant to the true believers. They showed great faith in their beliefs. As one testified, “we should trust our eyes and ears.” That’s instead of believing what science has shown.
Testimonials
The scientific facts don’t support those who opposed alcohol ads. So they were forced to rely on anecdotes, emotional appeals, impressions, beliefs, and extensive use of “junk science.” Of course there were testimonials, without which no tent meeting would be complete.
The true believers made great use of correlations. But these never, even once, proved anything. We know that increased consumption of ice cream is correlated with an increase in drownings. But that doesn’t mean that eating ice cream causes people to drown. People are more likely to both eat ice cream and to go swimming (and sometimes drown) in hot weather.
Virtually every true believer used meaningless correlations to convince legislators to impose more restrictions on ads. Some even called for the prohibition of alcohol ads. That reflected either naiveté or contempt for the Constitutionally guaranteed First Amendment free-speech rights of others.
Language




The junk science congregation tended to have its own vocabulary. Its meanings were different from the “outside world.” For example, much was made of alcohol ads appearing in youth-oriented magazines. To most people a youth-oriented magazine would have at least a majority of youthful readers. But to be clearly youth-oriented, perhaps the readership should be two-thirds young people, or perhaps three-fourths. Would you believe that anything above 15.8% youthful readership was defined as a youth-oriented magazine?!
This definition may be counter-intuitive. But if a common-sense definition were used the “researchers” wouldn’t have any headline-grabbing “findings” to report. That’s the nature of junk science. Those who practice it want sound bites instead of sound science.
The misuse of language to persuade was pervasive. For example, believers defined the term” binge” so loosely that a so-called binge drinker could be completely sober. And 20-year-old married adults serving their country in the military would be “kids.” Persuading others rather than presenting facts accurately is the goal of junk science.
True Believers
A the end of the day, the faithful alcohol junk science promoters went home. They returned to the Center for Addiction and Substance Abuse and the Center for Science in the Public Interest. To the Center on Alcohol Marketing and Youth, and other bastions of committed believers. There they could refresh their zeal to convert non-believers.
Resources: Alcohol Junk Science Promoters & Believers
Brignell, J. Sorry, Wrong Number! The Abuse of Measurement. London: Brignell, 2000.
Huff, D. How to Lie with Statistics. NY: Norton, 2010.
Milloy, S. Junk Science Judo: Self-Defense. Wash: Cato, 2001.
Milloy, S. J. Science Without Sense: Public Health Research. Wash: Cato, 1995.